Clinic & Spa Manager-Influencer Neuroenhancement-Physical Therapy-Ph.D. Biomedical Sciences-Aesthetic Medical-Sport Scientist-Neuroscience-Lagree Trainer
Biography:
Prof. in Human Perfomance, Human Movement Anatomy, PhD in Neuroscience, Research Psicology,Traumatology&Orthopedic, Project Manager , Strength and Conditioning specialist Physical Therapy,Skynesiology and Spinology, Exercise Physiology,Spa Therapist Physiology,Support Health Worker, aesthetic medical, sports masseur, expert in postural gymnastics, psychology and psychomotion for the elderly and special children or with learning disabilities.
Deep skills: positive mind neuroscience of exercise to counter Covid 19
Memory and Learning in the time of Covid 19
We must begin to lose our memory, even if only occasionally, to understand that memory is what fills our lives. Life without memory is not life. Our memory is our consistency, our reason, our feeling, even our action. Without her we are nothing.
Luis Bunuel
Memory is a wonderful mechanism, a means of transporting us back in time. We can go back a moment, or a large part of life. Sometimes not perfect, sometimes not authentic, sometimes with nuanced details, memory is however the system that allows us to recall the information we have stored and learned from both the external and internal environment. It is the experience that changes us, the contact with the environment that changes our behavior through a series of structural and functional changes in our nervous system. The last challenge of neuroscience is precisely to better understand the complexity of these mechanisms and how complex phenomena such as learning and memory can occur.
Although the changes that occur within the individual brain cells can be relatively simple, considering that the brain is made up of many billions of neurons, the overall phenomenon is certainly very complex and makes the isolation and identification of the specific changes responsible of a certain really difficult memory. Similarly, although the elements of a specific learning task may be simple, its implications for the organism can be very complex (Carlson, 2002).
From a neurobiological point of view, learning and memory are adaptations to the environment of the brain circuits that allow us to respond appropriately to situations we have previously experienced. Hence, learning (the process by which the nervous system acquires new information and experiences) and memory (the ability to retain, retain and recall this information) represent the main mechanisms through which environmental events shape behavior. The experiences are not simply "accumulated" in the brain, but are capable of causing plastic modifications in our nervous system and of altering the circuits involved in our most sophisticated functions; in this way they change the way we act, think, perceive, plan. Although learning and memory are two closely related functions, they have temporal sequences and nervous mechanisms that do not always coincide. From a neurophysiological point of view, in fact, the learning process is essentially functional, ie entrusted to reverberant circuits and unstable morphological modifications, while the memory process is structural and presupposes stable morphological modifications. These changes are accompanied by an increase in protein synthesis and consist of an increase in the number and volume of the dendritic spines (Gasbarri and Tomaz, 2005).
The importance of understanding the mechanisms that underlie the memory process, and the hope of being able to intervene when they are damaged, is therefore easily understood: they depend on the quality of life and the survival of the individual. This type of considerations helps to make the field of research on the mechanisms underlying memory particularly active. Many of these studies are carried out on animal models, but one wonders how much these studies are predictive of human reality both in terms of memory neurobiology (think, for example, of the different and more extensive development of the cortex in humans), and regarding the effect of drugs of potential therapeutic use in memory disorders. Some types of memory, however, have common characteristics in animals and humans, and animal models make it easier to study simple forms of learning and memory - such as the conditioned blink reflex - which are completely human similar to those studied in small mammals. Furthermore, the preliminary study on the animal very often allows to safeguard thousands of human lives and to avoid, for example, the experimentation of drugs on the ex-abducted man. Learning is the process by which experiences modify our nervous system and therefore our behavior. The primary function of the ability to learn is to develop behaviors suitable for a constantly changing environment. Experiences are able to change the way we perceive, act, think and plan. The ability of our nervous system to change in relation to experience is called synaptic plasticity and consists of a structural and functional change in the nervous structures which is reflected in changes in the processes of perception, memory, thought, planning and action.
Learning phenomena can be distinguished in:
Non-associative learning: habit and awareness
Associative learning: perceptual learning, stimulus response learning (classical / Pavlonian conditioning), instrumental / operative conditioning by trial and error, motor learning, relational learning. Although the understanding of the mechanisms responsible for learning and other plastic modifications that take place in the adult brain still remains the subject of debate, there is broad consensus on the concept that at the basis of these phenomena there are modifications finely regulated by the strength of the synapses (Madison et al., 1991).
While in development, the changes are based on the reorganization of the connections that already exist between the cells, the strength of the synapses can also change in adulthood. The cellular mechanisms underlying these changes are constituted by transient modifications of the synaptic neurotransmission processes, while in the case of more lasting modifications they are constituted by changes in the processes of gene expression. Even after development is complete, the ability to change the organization of the nervous circuits of the cortex is not completely lost, but this ability decreases considerably (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993).